Print

Print


April 25, 2000

Health Advice: A Matter of Cause, Effect and Confusion


It is a popular sport to joke that scientists change their minds from year
to year about what people should do to stay healthy. One year, we should
all be taking beta carotene supplements. The next year, the supplements
themselves seem to cause cancer. One year, it is high iron levels in the
blood that are causing heart disease. The next year it is low levels of
folic acid.

It becomes all too tempting to recall the scene from Woody Allen's movie
"Sleeper" in which the hero wakes up in the future and discovers that
science has decreed that all those health foods he had been forcing down
are actually injurious to health. The healthy foods are things like
sumptuous desserts and hamburgers. The bad foods are things like bran and
vegetables.

Predictably, references to "Sleeper" are starting again. It is not hard to
see why when, within weeks, data from large and rigorous studies have
thrown into question two long-held assumptions about preventive medicine.

First it was estrogen and heart disease. People knew -- or thought they did
-- that taking estrogen pills at menopause, when estrogen levels wane,
protected women with heart disease. People knew -- or thought they did --
that high-fiber diets protected against colon cancer.

Now, however, preliminary data from a large federal study indicate that at
least initially hormone replacement therapy is not protecting
postmenopausal women from heart disease and may even be leading to a tiny
increase in heart attacks, strokes and blood clots in the legs and lungs.
The data are far from conclusive, and few would conclude at this point that
estrogen was dangerous. But the point is that no one really knows whether
it protects against heart disease.

Last week, two large and rigorous federal studies of high-fiber diets
failed to show that they protected against colon cancer.

Once again, the results emphasize that this truth about diet and cancer is
far from established.

Scientists will say they knew it all along. If these truths were so
evident, there would be no need to do large studies in which people were
randomly assigned to take estrogen or, for comparison, to take a dummy
pill. There would be no need to do similar studies with high-fiber diets.
In fact, such studies would be unethical because they would deprive the
control groups of a valuable preventative measure.

The same goes for other questions now being investigated: does examining
the colon with sigmoidoscopy screening prevent colon cancer deaths? Does
the P.S.A. test and a digital rectal exam for prostate cancer save lives?
Does a high-fiber diet prevent heart disease?

Maybe the problem is one of language. Scientists and the public alike use
words like "prevents" and "protects against" and "lowers the risk of" when
they are discussing evidence that is suggestive, and hypothesis-generating,
as well as when they are discussing evidence that is as firm as science can
make it.

As reactions to the estrogen and high-fiber diet studies show, years of
such careless use of language can make preliminary evidence appear to be
ironclad.

"It's one of the biggest problems in this business," said Dr. Gilbert S.
Omenn, a public health expert who is executive vice president for medical
affairs at the University of Michigan.

Yet even in the aftermath of the high-fiber diet studies, researchers were
speaking confidently about other measures people could take to "prevent"
colon cancer, like exercising and staying thin. And they were saying that
there were reasons to keep eating fiber because it could "reduce the risk"
of heart disease.

When asked about the evidence for these statements, the researchers
confessed that it was, of course, the lower level hypothesis-generating kind.

Dr. Omenn came across this problem after a study he directed, and he has
some advice. Dr. Omenn's large and rigorous study asked whether beta
carotene, the ingredient in fruits and vegetables that many thought
protected against cancer and heart disease, could, in fact, protect people.
He found that not only were beta carotene supplements ineffective but they
actually increased the risk of lung cancer in smokers. His findings, and
those of others who found the same thing, were met with shock, denial and
dismay.

In an article published in 1995 in Annals of Epidemiology, Dr. Omenn urged
scientists to be more careful about the words they used. "We should be
fastidious in referring strictly to 'associations' when the studies have
generated associations; we should reserve the terms 'effects,'
'reductions,' 'decreases' and 'protects' to studies that actually alter
incidences of end points, preferably in randomized, double-masked designs,"
he wrote.

"That's exactly right," said Dr. Barnett Kramer, deputy director of the
division of cancer prevention at the National Cancer Institute. As editor
of The Journal of the National Cancer Institute, Dr. Kramer deals directly
with authors whose words convey more certainty than their data.

"We often have to work with them to change the wording," Dr. Kramer said.
The ambiguous language is understandable, Dr. Kramer added, because
scientists studying preventive measures fervently want their findings to be
meaningful.

"It's easy to look at your data and say, 'Gee, I believe it has strong
implications for human health,' " Dr. Kramer said. "It takes a lot of
discipline to slow down and say, 'There are a lot of steps between what I
observed and what I hope is true.'"


By GINA KOLATA
Copyright 2000 The New York Times Company
"http://www.nytimes.com/library/national/science/health/042500hth-health-adv
ice.html"

janet paterson
53 now / 41 dx / 37 onset
a new voice: http://www.geocities.com/janet313/
613 256 8340 PO Box 171 Almonte Ontario Canada K0A 1A0